Lazy, filthy pigs? Think again

Posted on 08 December 2016 by

Gemma Carder

in the Animal sentience blog

Pigs are often associated with negative labels such as ‘filthy’, ‘lazy’ and ‘greedy’. But why is the word ‘pig’ used as an insult, and is there any truth to it?

The reality beyond the stereotypes

Science shows pigs are emotional, smart, inquisitive animals, who engage in a range of complex behaviours. When you look at it this way - they don’t deserve to be connected with commonly-used insults.

Contrary to the idea that pigs are lazy - some of them are keen swimmers. In the Bahamas there is a small, uninhabited island where wild pigs enjoy swimming in the clear waters.

Sensitive, social creatures

Pigs are social animals. Research shows they enjoy playing together [1]  (which can even be encouraged by music [2]), are empathetic, and are affected by the emotions of other pigs  [3].

Subscribe to our newsletterSign up

We have a local office in Africa. Please sign up through our Africa website instead.

We have a local office in Australia. Please sign up through our Australia website instead.

We have an office in Brazil. Please sign up through our Brazil website instead.

We have an office in Canada. Please sign up through our Canada website instead.

We have a local office in China. Please sign up through our China website instead.

We have a local office in Denmark. Please sign up through our Denmark website instead.

We have a local office in India. Please sign up through our India website instead.

We have a local office in Latin America. Please sign up through our Latin America website instead.

We have a local office in the Netherlands. Please sign up through our Netherlands website instead.

We have a local office in New Zealand. Please sign up through our New Zealand website instead.

We have a local office in Sweden. Please sign up through our Sweden website instead.

We have a local office in Thailand. Please sign up through our Thailand website instead.

We have a local office in the UK. Please sign up through our UK website instead.

We have a local office in the US. Please sign up through our US website instead.

World Animal Protection will not sell or swap your information with any third party. If you’d like to stop hearing from us, or change the way we communicate, please email info@worldanimalprotection.org. For information on how we use your details, and how we keep your details safe, please read our privacy policy.

They have been seen to wag their tails in response to seeing mates experience something positive, and hold their ears back when other pigs experience something negative [3]. 

Piglets are attached to their mothers and are known to scream in distress when they are separated.

Pigs are very smart, and can even use mirrors to assess their environment. When pigs were shown a reflection of their food bowl, they used the reflection to locate their food [5].

They are often seen as filthy, but pigs actually use mud as sunscreen to protect their delicate skin, which is prone to burning in the sun.

The complex needs of pigs are often not met on farms

Pigs descended from the Eurasian wild boar, and there is little difference in the behavioural needs of domestic pigs from those of their wild ancestors [6]. For example, they need space and the opportunity to forage to be happy and healthy.

In poor welfare farming systems, pigs often do not have the chance to behave in the way they need to, for example playing and foraging.

Studies show pigs housed in close confinement with one another are more aggressive [7], and often demonstrate their mental suffering through bar and tail biting.

These behaviours usually occur when pigs are frustrated, bored and have a lack of control over their surroundings.

The farming industry must consider pigs’ needs

Regardless of negative associations with pigs, research has proven that they are no different to many other animals, including the pets we share our homes with.

Pigs feel a range of complex emotions, and can suffer both physically and mentally.

We’re working with companies in some of the biggest pork producing countries to promote higher welfare farming alternatives, such as indoor group housing and free-range farming. We want to see a world where every pig farm is designed around the welfare needs of the animals.

Read about how we’re improving the lives of 70 billion farm animals.

Find out more

1. Martin, J. E.; Ison, S. H.; Baxter, E. M. The influence of neonatal environment on piglet play behaviour and post-weaning social and cognitive development. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 163, 69–79.

2. de Jonge, F. H.; Boleij, H.; Baars, A. M.; Dudink, S.; Spruijt, B. M. Music during play-time: Using context conditioning as a tool to improve welfare in piglets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 115, 138–148.

3. Reimert, I.; Bolhuis, J. E.; Kemp, B.; Rodenburg, T. B. Emotions on the loose: emotional contagion and the role of oxytocin in pigs. Anim. Cogn. 2015, 18, 517–532.

4. Iacobucci, P.; Colonnello, V.; D’Antuono, L.; Cloutier, S.; Newberry, R. C. Piglets call for maternal attention: Vocal behaviour in Sus scrofa domesticus is modulated by mother’s proximity. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 88–93.

5. Broom, D. M.; Sena, H.; Moynihan, K. L. Pigs learn what a mirror image represents and use it to obtain information. Anim. Behav. 2009, 78, 1037–1041.

6. Mendl, M.; Held, S.; Byrne, R. W. Pig cognition. Curr. Biol. 2010, 20, 796–798.

7. Greenwood, E. C.; Plush, K. J.; van Wettere, W. H. E. J.; Hughes, P. E. Hierarchy formation in newly mixed, group housed sows and management strategies aimed at reducing its impact. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 160, 1–11.

More posts from:

Gemma Carder

Tell the world:

LinkedIn